The wiki has recently been updated. Please contact me by talk page or email if you encounter any issues.

Middlesex

From FrathWiki
Jump to navigationJump to search

The Middlesex language was the common speech register of the early Crystals, used in written communication. Men and women pronounced the words differently, and in their spoken language, they also used different vocabulary and syntax that were not written down except in quotations. Children also had their own speech register.

Scratchpad

See Icecap and possibly a new scratchpad for the combined project.

May 18, 2021

Women may use yu- instead of *yʷa- or just /ya-/ for the handheld classifier prefix, as if it had come from /yu-/ all along/.


Since there are three different sounds, at least one of them may in fact become p in at least some words, likely due to morphological conditioning rather than traditional sound change.

It is possible that one more consonant may drop, creating new vowel sequences, but this would be more likely if earlier sequences like èa and òa (that is, sequences with no high vowels) shifted to something else first.

The primordial suffix -hə̀(-n), meaning "of the womb", disappears in Middlesex by aspirating the preceding consonant (even over a vowel) and shifting the tone of that vowel to low. Thus ŋùbu "torch" ---> ŋupŭ, and so on. The stress may shift back to the first syllable through analogy. Note that this never overlaps with the genitive because the genitive always creates a high tone on the last syllable instead.

The addition of a nearly meaningless -a suffix can turn many words into male/female pairs, since if the stem of the word ended with any high vowel it would create a contrast. It may be commonly assumed to have been /-u/ even if evidence indicates otherwise.

Adolescents and children

The word "teenager, adolescent" may shift in meaning to small children and become to them what -ik is for women. Men would still not have a convenient suffix of their own and may simply have the unmarked forms of nouns since they would be set apart by their speech styles. Note that these are intended to mark indefinite nouns .... that is, "the woman's towel" with a classifier prefix vs "a towel for women" with a suffix. Both the consonant and the vowel in bò would themselves change depending on speech register.

Though etymologically unsound, bò could be simply replaced by bə̀ at an early stage of the language, which would cause vowel mutation and then disappear. (the shifts would be a e i o u > ō ō ū ō ū, indifferent to tone.) These shifts are early enough that the later gender splits would not be able to piece them apart, and therefore men would pronounce all five of the resulting morphemes as /ū/ and effectively lose the final vowel on all words ending with this diminutive suffix. Meanwhile women would have /ʷā ʷā ū ʷā ū/.

May 16, 2021

Feminine property suffix

Note the persistence of the suffix -ik for feminine property in an otherwise primarily CV language. This creates vowel alternations, for example turning final /-a/ into /-ek/, which in turn is pronounced [ik] by women and [ʲak] by men.

However, -e and -o probably just stay -ek & -ok. There may be some other words that restore lost final consonants.

Borrowing between registers

When men and women borrow each other's words, they convert the consonants to their own gender's forms, but not the vowels, and in some words the changes are not reversible. For example, women have just a single phoneme corresponding to men's and , and women additionally also pronounce k as before a historical /o/ vowel, because for women /o/ is /ʷa/.

Men use women's words for female property, deriving from women's own pronunciations of those words. (e.g. "my towel", etc) This leads to consonant alternations affecting the stem of the word. Likewise, all words containing an o on any tone shift it to u on that tone when referring to a man's property; this applies to both classifiers and the stems.

To some extent, the "levelled" pronunciation may win out; men pronounce both /o/ and /u/ as u, but women also have an /u/, so a woman pronouncing an /u/ can still be correct. This leads the degenerate (in the sense of levelled) pronunciations to dominate over correct and hypercorrect ones. Since men cannot pronounce an /o/, they cannot be hypercorrect and cannot restore /o/ in any way to a word that has been generalized to /u/. This does not usually apply to /e/ because women pronounce a distinction between /e/ and /i/ in most words (depending on the preceding consonant).

On the other hand, note that men can still pronounce /ʷa/, which is how women pronounce /o/, for most words (depending on the preceding consonant).

Evolution

See also Tropical_Rim#Tapilula_.280.29_to_Star_Empire_Amade_.281900.29.

Tapilula (0) to Star Empire Amade (1900)

It is possible that a branch of this family ends up in Kxesh; see Gold_Empire#Migration. Despite the Stars' homeland being described as Lobexon, it is likely that most migrants were from Amade.

  1. Accented schwas surrendered their accent to the following vowel (not the same as a stress shift, because the tone also changes).
  2. The "labial" vowel ə disappeared, syllabified nearby consonants or turned to i if the nearby consonants were not possible to become syllabic. Note that it never occurred after labialized consonants. Sequences such as /kəh hək/ (that is, in either original order) collapsed to form aspirated consonants, though these behaved as clusters.
  3. Tautosyllabic vowel sequences òi ài èi converged to ē. This did not affect syllable-straddling words like /tùya/. Likewise, èu àu òu in the same environment converged to ō.
    In some cases, the result of ài may instead be a short o, due to memories of a still-active morpheme compounding process.
  4. Duplicate vowel sequences àa èe ìi òo ùu shifted to long vowels ā ē ī ō ū. But the same sequences with the opposite tone pattern did not shift.
  5. The sequences ṁg ṅg ŋ̇g shifted to ṁb ṅd ŋ̇ġ.
  6. The velar fricatives g gʷ shifted to Ø w.
  7. The labial stops p b merged as b.
  8. In a closed syllable, the stops b t d shifted to w Ø Ø and lengthened the preceding vowel. New ēw āw ōw merged as ō, while new īw ūw merged as ū.
  9. Word-final became k. Word-final h spread across the preceding vowel; in other clusters, the /h/ transposed across the syllable boundary to form an aspirated consonant. (This is a general shift that had occurred near the beginning of the history, but it needed to happen again due to compounding.)
  10. The sequences bh dh shifted to p t.
  11. The labialized consonants tʷ dʷ nʷ shifted to kʷ v m. However the rare cluster ndʷ instead became mb.
  12. The labial fricative f shifted to h.
  13. The velar ejective shifted to g.
  14. Before a hiatus, the short vowels u i shifted to ʷ y, creating a new set of labialized consonants. However, the palatalized consonants were not distinct from their components. IT IS POSSIBLE THAT /h/ WAS TRANSPARENT TO THIS. THIS IS NOT YET ON THE OTHER LIST!!!!!
  15. The sequences tʲ nʲ dʲ lʲ shifted to č ň ž y. Then kʲ ŋʲ hʲ shifted to č ň š. Palatalized labials depalatalized.

Thus the consonant inventory was

                  PLAIN                    LABIALIZED
Bilabials:        p   m       b   v        pʷ  mʷ      bʷ  w
Alveolars:        t   n       d   l        tʷ  nʷ      dʷ  lʷ
Palataloids:      č   ň   š   ž   y         
Velars:           k   ŋ   h   g  (Ø)       kʷ  ŋʷ  hʷ  gʷ

And the vowels were /a e i o u ā ē ī ō ū/.

Star Empire Crystal (1900) to Middlesex Baeba Crystal (3370)

  1. All aspirated sonorants deaspirated. (This may have happened earlier in history.)
    Consider not doing this, as it was important in the grammar.
  2. The voiceless fricatives h hʷ shifted to x h.
  3. The fricatives d dʷ ž shifted to r w y.
  4. Then the postalveolar fricative š (always from /hʲ/) shifted to s. This change operated on the surface level, meaning that, for example, men's realization of /si/ was still [si], not [sʲi] as one would expect from the pattern set by the stops.
  5. The consonant clusters ll nn, which occurred primarily in loanwords, shifted to ḷ ṇ.
  6. The labialized coronals nʷ lʷ shifted to ṇ ḷ. The stop came to be spelled ; its pronunciation split between the male and female registers with neither side retaining the original pronunciation.
  7. The nasals mʷ ŋʷ shifted to m̄ ŋ̄.
  8. Any shifted to y; note, however, that in many cases the sequence would have been analogized away instead of shifting.

If the vowel sequence òa passed unchanged to Middlesex, it may have become ʷā in the speech of both men and women. If it were smoothed out earlier on, it would behave as an ordinary long ō, being pronounced /ʷā/ by women and /ū/ by men.



Both men and women had a three-vowel surface inventory of /a i u/, but because they had shifted the inherited /a e i o u/ to three vowels in different ways, none of the vowel changes were part of the shared language, and thus none were represented in the orthography. And because the consonant changes were all unconditional, the parent language spellings were all still understandable to the Crystals, and all changes in the preceding 1500 years could be attributed to spelling. Thus Crystal scholars sometimes considered their language to have been unchanged for the preceding 1900 years.[1]

Phonology

Consonant inventory

The written language consonant inventory was


Middlesex.png

Idiolectal variation

Symmetrical variations

Women pronounced hʷ~bʷ as /hʷ/ and men pronounced it as /bʷ/. WOmen pronounced f~b as /f/ and men pronounced it as /b/. Women pronounced xʷ~gʷ as /xʷ/ and men pronounced it as /gʷ/.

Female mergers

Women pronounced ṭ g v as /kʷ x f/.

In a few words, /s/ was hʷ before /a/ for women (thgese words had /o/ for men).

Vowels

Middlesex writing used a five-vowel script, but both men and women used a three vowel inventory, /a i u/, in their speech. The five vowel symbols were needed to tell apart which words varied by gender and which did not.


For men, the five vowel symbols a e i o u spell a ʲa ʲi u u.

For women, the five vowel symbols a e i o u spell a i ʲi ʷa u.

The /u/ may actually be /ʷu/, but this does not create a distinction between the final two vowels for men.



Separate gender sound changes

NOTE, THIS SECTION IS * COPY-PASTED *, NOT LINKED, FROM Tropical Rim.

Star Empire Amade (1900) to proto-Feminine (2643)

The Star party takes control of Amade in the year 2643, so it is likely that the Oysters never completely wiped out the Stars' languages in this region. But note that these were not the aboriginal Star languages, but rather the ones that had been introduced by migrants in the pre-political era.

This branch may be the dialect of the all-female nation of Katalapa. It is also possible that the Crystals re-homogenize Amade's linguistic diversity when they take over, since Crystals needed to travel between nations frequently.

Note that this branch has /ʷi/.

The initial consonant inventory was

                  PLAIN                    LABIALIZED
Bilabials:        p   m       b   v        pʷ  mʷ      bʷ  w
Alveolars:        t   n       d   l        tʷ  nʷ      dʷ  lʷ
Palataloids:      č   ň   š   ž   y         
Velars:           k   ŋ   h   g  (Ø)       kʷ  ŋʷ  hʷ  gʷ

And the vowels were /a e i o u ā ē ī ō ū/.

  1. The voiceless fricatives h hʷ shifted to x h.
  2. The voiced sounds v b bʷ g shifted to f f fʷ x.
  3. The fricatives d dʷ ž shifted to r w y.
  4. Then the postalveolar fricative š shifted to s.
  5. The vowel o shifted to ʷo (phonetically /ʷa/).
  6. The mid front vowel e shifted to i, making palatalization phonemic. Thus only three vowels remained.
  7. The consonant clusters ll nn, which occurred primarily in loanwords, shifted to ḷ ṇ.
  8. The fricative shifted to .
  9. The labialized coronals tʷ nʷ sʷ lʷ shifted to kʷ ṇ hʷ ḷ.
  10. The nasals mʷ ŋʷ shifted to m̄ ŋ̄.

Thus the consonant inventory was

Rounded bilabials:     pʷ      hʷ  w       
Spread bilabials:      p   m   f           m̄
Dentals:                   ṇ       ḷ
Alveolars:             t   n   s   l   r
Palataloids:           č   ň       y
Velars:                k   ŋ   x           ŋ̄
Labiovelars:           kʷ      xʷ  
Postvelars:                    h

Star Empire Amade (1900) to proto-Masculine (2643)

Note that this branch has /ʷi/. The naming is in the intent that this is the language of the masculine Crystal nations, but realistically it is more likely that the Crystals homogenize the languages of the male and female nations because citizens of each traveled to the opposite side to find mates. Two reasons why it is difficult to assign these languages to female and male populations separately are:

  1. The Crystals may not have organized their population that way for nearly a thousand years after the evolution of these two branches of the family.
  2. It is not clear that the Crystals arose in Amade to begin with.

If this is a Crystal language at all, it must have a maturation date much later than 2643.

The initial consonant inventory was

                  PLAIN                    LABIALIZED
Bilabials:        p   m       b   v        pʷ  mʷ      bʷ  w
Alveolars:        t   n       d   l        tʷ  nʷ      dʷ  lʷ
Palataloids:      č   ň   š   ž   y         
Velars:           k   ŋ   h   g  (Ø)       kʷ  ŋʷ  hʷ  gʷ


  1. The front vowels e i shifted to ʲa ʲi.
  2. The mid back vowel o shifted to u.
  3. The voiced sounds d dʷ ž shifted to r w y.
  4. The voiceless fricatives h hʷ shifted to x h.
  5. The consonant clusters ll nn, which occurred primarily in loanwords, shifted to ḷ ṇ.
  6. The sequences tʷ lʷ nʷ shifted to ṭ ḷ ṇ.
  7. The sequence ʷi shifted to i.
    TENTATIVE
  8. The voiceless postalveolar fricative š shifted to s.
  9. The nasals mʷ ŋʷ shifted to m̄ n̄ ŋ̄.
    This language is well to the east of Pabahais, so these may be dummy sounds that later merge into /m/.

Thus the consonant inventory was

Rounded bilabials: pʷ          bʷ  w
Spread bilabials:  p   m       b   v   m̄
Dentals:           ṭ   ṇ           ḷ
Alveolars:         t   n   s   r   l
Palataloids:       č   ň           y
Velars:            k   ŋ   x   g       ŋ̄
Labiovelars:       kʷ          gʷ
Postvelars:                h


It is possible that the palataloids could be analyzed away if assuming /tia ti/ = [ča či] and /ki/ = [ki] (if /ʷi/ > /i/).

The voiceless bilabial stop /p/, and possibly certain other consonants, are very rare in citation forms, appearing only through consonant mutation as in the distantly related Andanic languages. The velar stop /k/ is affected by the same situation as /p/ but only in medial position. The hiatus changes might also be very rare since they, too, were grammatically conditioned. The language could also perform analogy in reverse and delete consonants that are etymologically justified.

Adult speech registers

Men and women both substitute r for any v (which women would ordinarily pronounce as /f/) in certain words, but the pattern is unpredictable and it typically does not cover the same words. Thus, women do not often have minimal pairs where men distinguish /v/ and /f/. This is possible because the phonemes that later shifted to /r/ and /v/ had been in grammatical alternations early on. Note, though, that this is missing the final vowel.

Similarly, k can substitute for , for a similar reason, also losing the final vowel.



Women's speech

Women have a smaller consonant inventory than men, leading to minimal pairs that must be disambiguated through classifier prefixes or, less commonly, by using a different word entirely. For example, where men distinguish /nàga/ "voice" from /nàxa/ "owl", women pronounce both as /nàxa/

Men's speech

Children's speech

Children use the simple vowels /a i u/, without coarticulations. All words have consonant harmony, containing a maximum of three different consonants. They also have vowel harmony, containing a maximum of two vowels; [u] is considered a variant of /i/ and submissive to it. Nevertheless, there may still be five vowels in the spelling, as they behave in a similar manner to how adults' vowels would.

Scratchpad (Gĭri)

May 31, 2021

The consonant inventory could in fact be much smaller than that given below, if assuming children used a vocabulary of only around 100 words, and any use of words outside this inventory was an instance of loaning from adult speech registers. This would help the language tie in with Moonshine. However, Moonshine could also have shed its inherited Middlesex children's words.

Possible consonant inventory:


Bilabials:                   m   b
Coronals:                t   n   l
Palatals:                        y
Velars:                  kʷ  ŋ  (Ø)

May 18, 2021

Scope

Children use Gĭri unbroken by adult mannerisms until the age of five, both among themselves and with adults. For children to adopt adult speech patterns before age five is considered to be putting oneself above one's peers. Accidental use of adult speech is common, and not punished; only when it becomes a pattern will adults see this as misbehavior on par with an adult using the opposite gender's register.

Choice of consonants

The consonant preceding the stressed syllable is the primary thematic consonant, and the consonant in the other syllable (assuming Gĭri roots cannot be more than 2 syllables) is the secondary. Classifiers will shift to the primary at least partially .... that is, /ŋ/ may become /m/ as it moves halfway to /b/ .... and this could be the tertiary consonant.


May 16, 2021

Children could use the prefix ŋa- for large and dangerous objects, and ŋe- as a prefix of respect for adults. (This would be pronounced with /a/ by boys and with /i/ by girls.)

If a cognate of the Trout 2P respectful pronoun /ogĭtʷo/ is found here, it could potentially lose its first three phonemes all to reanalysis, thus becoming just /tʷò/, which would appear in Middlesex as kʷò, and this in turn would be either /kʷà/ or /kʷù/ depending on gender.

Pronunciation of some sounds could filter upward into adult speech registers. Since children can only use three consonants per word, content morphemes might dominate prefixes and suffixes, and lead to harmony.

Possible consonant inventory:

Rounded bilabials:       pʷ  m̄   w
Spread bilabials:        p   m   b
Coronals:                t   n   l
Palatals:                        y
Velars:                  k   ŋ   g
Labiovelars:             kʷ  ŋ̄

There were only three vowels. There may have also been a pair of voiceless fricatives /s h/, or just /h/.

These may be spelled with syllable glyphs instead, as the syllables were likely pure CV, even disallowing prenasals. Children may omit classifier prefixes when referring to their own property, using them as if they refer by default to other people's property. Thus "my crib, my cradle" with no prefix alongside obù "crib, cradle" which refers to any crib not belonging to the speaker.

Notes

  1. not 1500, because they used a different start date.